By Laurie Niles: What is the best way to memorize music? And how can the science of the brain help us be more effective with memorization?Molly Gebrian -- who is an expert in both viola and neuroscience -- is the perfect person to investigate the possibilities, which is exactly what she did in a lecture entitled "The Brain and Memorization" at the 2018 American Viola Society Festival at The Colburn School.![Molly Gebrian]()
Molly Gebrian and her memorization strategies.
One important consideration is the fact that memory is multi-faceted and complex, Gebrian said. When a person says something like, "I have a bad memory" or "I have a good memory," this makes memory sound like a singular quality. In reality, memory has many dimensions.
"There are different kinds of memory that are independent of each other," Gebrian said. Simply speaking, those different kinds include long-term memory, short-term memory and working memory. Long-term memory includes memory for facts and events in your life, as well as "muscle memory." Short-term memory is for remembering things such as phone numbers. Working memory includes memories that are held in one's mind while manipulating that information. One example for musicians of working memory would be remembering accidentals in a piece of music while sight reading it.
How are long-term memories made? Getting information into one's brain involves something called "encoding." The deeper the encoding, the better the memory. "If you don't encode well, you won't remember the information as well."
Getting information from short-term memory into long-term memory is called "consolidation," and sleep is critical to this process. Getting the information back out of the brain is called "retrieval."
"Every time you retrieve that information - or even try to retrieve it -- you consolidate that information better," Gebrian said.
Something happens when you consolidate information: you are able to remember certain patterns of things in "chunks," and that is less work on your brain. For example, when learning to read, we start by sounding out individual letters, but eventually we learn syllables and even words, and we are able to identify these things much faster and with much less work. This kind of memory consolidation is called "chunking." The same thing happens in music.
Forming memory "chunks" turns out to be very important in memorizing music.
For example, the beginner learning May Song in Suzuki Book 1 may be learning it note-by-note, but a more advanced student or player would recognize its opening as a simple arpeggio. Practicing scales, arpeggios and etudes creates those familiar "chunks."
"This is the point of practicing technique: creating motor chunks and auditory memory chunks," Gebrian said. "If we don't practice technique, we don't have chunks. Practicing technique is not a waste of time at all; it aids our memory."
Music theory also gives us "chunks" having to do with the overall structure of a piece of music, its chord progressions and other concepts that help memory.
A beginner or novice has no chunks. And what happens when you have little or no chunks? It means that your working memory has to be very active, piecing together every small detail from scratch. This is not the best scenario for performing a piece of music by memory.
"You don't want to have a lot of working memory going when you are on stage," Gebrian pointed out. Working memory tends to be very sensitive to pressure, whereas using long-term memory is much less taxing on the brain.
A better player, who has "chunked" portions of the music, requires less short-term "working memory" and uses more long-term memory. The expert-level player has even bigger chains of chunks called "knowledge structures," which rely even more on long-term memory less on working memory.
When it comes to music, there are many different aspects of a piece that one can commit to memory: its form; its expressive characteristics; the rhythm and dynamics; the basic techniques involved. Memorizing involves remembering a combination of these different things, but which are the most important and effective?
Gebrian used the example of a psychology professor named Roger Chaffin, from the University of Connecticut, who did a study on memorization with professional musicians. He had each musician videotape every practice session leading up to a performance, giving commentary on their practice and why they practiced various ways. These were categorized into four types of performance cues: structural cues, about the form of a piece; expressive cues, about the emotional content; interpretive cues, about tempo and dynamics; and basic cues, about technique such as fingerings, etc. He then contacted each musician two years after their performance and asked them to write down the piece they had played. The question he was trying to answer was: which of these cues proved the most durable over time, for memory? As it turned out, the structural and expressive cues were the most durable. The technical cues were actually the least durable.
Gebrian's takeaway: "You need to have a lot of different cues throughout the pieces that jog memory."
These cues play on three types of memory: muscle memory, auditory memory and declarative memory -- "You have to have all three, and they all enforce each other," Gebrian said.
Here are a few practice techniques that Gebrian suggested for enforcing each kind of memory:

Molly Gebrian and her memorization strategies.
- Muscle memory: Practice "air viola" or "air violin" to isolate just the muscle movement. Also, try practicing on a de-tuned instrument, to dissociate the muscle action from the sound coming from the instrument.
- Auditory memory (sound memory): Sing the piece from memory. To add a level to that, sing out loud while fingering silently.
- Declarative memory (memory of structure): Draw a formal diagram of the piece; and/or write out the piece from memory.
- Identify performance cues
- Get enough sleep
- Practice what you're memorizing last thing at night and first thing in the morning
- Play from memory early in the process
- Randomize (interleave) your practice, change it up to test memory
- Think about sound, expression and phrasing while playing from memory, more than technique and specific notes
- Video yourself playing from memory
- Perform by memory for others before the big concert